The Myth of Mozart’s Sight-Reading Genius

A Closer Examination

In the 18th century, exaggerated accounts of Mozart’s abilities were as common as they were fantastical. Yet, detailed reports from figures like Daines Barrington and André Grétry reveal a far more human side of the prodigy, whose skills, impressive though they were, did not amount to the miraculous feats often attributed to him.

Mozart: The Fall of the Gods

This book offers a fresh and critical look at the life of Wolfgang Amadeus Mozart, challenging the myths that have surrounded him for centuries. We strip away the romanticised image of the “natural genius” and delve into the contradictions within Mozart’s extensive biographies. Backed by nearly 2,000 meticulously sourced citations, this work invites readers to explore a deeper, more complex understanding of Mozart. Perfect for those who wish to question the traditional narrative, this biography is a must-read for serious music lovers and historians.

"Even the most lauded prodigies sometimes hide a kernel of reality beneath layers of myth."

Mozart: The Fall of the Gods

Separating Fact from Fiction in 18th-Century Reports

In the concert posters promoting young Wolfgang Mozart, he was lauded as a prodigy capable of sight-reading even the most difficult pieces flawlessly. Such claims, often more fantastical than credible, warrant closer scrutiny. Two notable contemporary accounts diverge from the usual paternal praise or the laudatory articles seen in 18th-century newspapers. One is a scientific report from London, penned by Daines Barrington (1727–1800), a member of the Royal Society. The report, dating from June 1765, was finalized in 1769 and presented on February 15, 1770, a full five years after the original test.

Barrington was a multifaceted figure: a barrister, antiquarian, and naturalist with a penchant for ornithology and a fascination for musical prodigies. His book, a curious collection of studies ranging from the origins of turkeys to the melodiousness of birds, often employed flowery and exaggerated language. Perhaps Barrington’s fondness for child prodigies contributed to the surge in popularity of such marvels in England. He documented not only the Mozarts but also other youthful musicians like the Wesley brothers and William Crotch. Yet, his approach lacked scientific rigour, often relying on hearsay and questionable sources.

Barrington’s report on Mozart was no exception. In his experiment, he presented a manuscript duet, its composer unspecified, to test Mozart’s sight-reading skills. The piece was from Metastasio’s Demofoonte, arranged for two vocal parts and strings. Curiously, the vocal parts were written in the alto clef, which Barrington highlighted as if it were a major obstacle. Despite this build-up, Wolfgang executed the symphony introduction “masterfully,” as Barrington put it, then sang the upper vocal line while his father Leopold tackled the lower. Leopold faltered, eliciting disapproving looks from his son, who even corrected his mistakes. Barrington claimed that Wolfgang not only sang beautifully but also embellished the accompaniment with violin parts to enhance the musical effect.

A closer analysis reveals that the test was calibrated for both Mozarts, and Wolfgang, not surprisingly, outperformed his father. Stripping away the superlatives, we see that Wolfgang competently played the bass line with his left hand and added occasional violin parts with his right. Impressive for an eight-year-old, certainly, but not the miracle Barrington made it out to be. His comparison of the duet’s difficulty to reading ancient texts like Etruscan and Greek reveals his musical naivety. Instead of a supernatural feat, the performance was a well-practiced skill honed by Leopold to impress the general public.

Even more telling is a different account from André Ernest Modeste Grétry (1741–1813), a professional musician who tested Mozart in Geneva. When Leopold boasted of his son’s sight-reading prowess, Grétry composed a challenging sonata movement in B-flat major. Mozart played it, yet replaced several difficult passages with his own improvisations. The audience, except Grétry, was in awe. Grétry’s skepticism highlighted Wolfgang’s reliance on improvisation and lack of systematic study, a strategy that gave the illusion of mastery.

Mozart’s ability to read music, while exceptional for his age, fell short of true sight-reading as it’s defined today: a flawless, spontaneous execution of written music, requiring years of rigorous training and experience. Grétry’s observations dispel the myth of Mozart’s innate genius, pointing instead to his reliance on quick-thinking and a boldness typical of children.

 

You May Also Like

Mozart’s K 71: A Fragment Shrouded in Doubt and Uncertainty

Mozart’s K 71: A Fragment Shrouded in Doubt and Uncertainty

Mozart’s K 71, an incomplete aria, is yet another example of musical ambiguity. The fragment’s authorship, dating, and even its very existence as a genuine Mozart work remain open to question. With no definitive evidence, how can this fragment be so confidently attributed to him?

Unpacking Mozart’s Early Education

Unpacking Mozart’s Early Education

The story of Ligniville illustrates the pitfalls of romanticizing Mozart’s early life and education, reminding us that the narrative of genius is often a construct that obscures the laborious aspects of musical development.

The False Sonnet of Corilla Olimpica

The False Sonnet of Corilla Olimpica

Leopold Mozart’s relentless pursuit of fame for his son Wolfgang led to questionable tactics, including fabricating a sonnet by the renowned poetess Corilla Olimpica. This desperate attempt to elevate Wolfgang’s reputation casts a shadow over the Mozart legacy.

Mozart’s K 73A: A Mystery Wrapped in Ambiguity

Mozart’s K 73A: A Mystery Wrapped in Ambiguity

K.143 is a prime example of how Mozart scholarship has turned uncertainty into myth. With no definitive evidence of authorship, date, or purpose, this uninspired recitative and aria in G major likely originated elsewhere. Is it time to admit this is not Mozart’s work at all?

A Farce of Honour in Mozart’s Time

A Farce of Honour in Mozart’s Time

By the time Wolfgang Amadeus Mozart received the Speron d’Oro, the once esteemed honour had become a laughable trinket, awarded through networking and influence rather than merit. Far from reflecting his musical genius, the title, shared with figures like Casanova, symbolised ridicule rather than respect.

The Legend of Mozart’s Miserere

The Legend of Mozart’s Miserere

The enduring popularity of the narrative surrounding Mozart’s Miserere highlights the allure of the prodigy myth, but as we peel back the layers, we uncover a more nuanced picture of his life and the musical landscape of the time. The reality often contrasts sharply with the romanticized tales that have shaped our understanding of his genius.